Affiliations 

  • 1 School of Psychology, Victoria University of Wellington, Kelburn, Wellington 6012, New Zealand; School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, Queensland 4067, Australia; Department of Psychology, Sunway University, Bandar Sunway 47500 Selangor Darul Ehsan, Malaysia. Electronic address: frankie.fong@vuw.ac.nz
  • 2 School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, Queensland 4067, Australia
  • 3 School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, Queensland 4067, Australia; School of Psychology, The University of Adelaide, Adelaide, South Australia 5005, Australia
J Exp Child Psychol, 2025 Feb 19;253:106205.
PMID: 39978308 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2025.106205

Abstract

We adapted a previous protocol to assess children's ability to spontaneously associate a novel cause with a novel emotional expression. An experimenter opened a series of boxes and generated an expression based on what was inside (the cause of the emotion). Participants (4- to 9-year-olds; N = 72) guessed what the experimenter saw from four possible objects linked to four expressions: stickers (happy), a broken balloon (sad), a spider (scared), and a novel object, pax (novel puffed cheeks expression). Children were then invited to open a series of boxes and generate expressions for the experimenter. Results suggest that children used a process of elimination to associate the novel pax object with the puffed cheeks expression. Some children also re-produced the puffed cheeks expression in a later task. As a final trial, when children were asked how people would feel when seeing the pax object, younger children tended to use positive labels and older children used negative labels. These results show that children are able to quickly associate novel facial expressions with precipitating events as early as 4 years of age, comparable to their performance in linking familiar expressions and objects.

* Title and MeSH Headings from MEDLINE®/PubMed®, a database of the U.S. National Library of Medicine.